Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Modulation of social interactions by immune stimulation in honey bee, Apis mellifera, workers

Abstract

Background

Immune response pathways have been relatively well-conserved across animal species, with similar systems in both mammals and invertebrates. Interestingly, honey bees have substantially reduced numbers of genes associated with immune function compared with solitary insect species. However, social species such as honey bees provide an excellent environment for pathogen or parasite transmission with controlled environmental conditions in the hive, high population densities, and frequent interactions. This suggests that honey bees may have developed complementary mechanisms, such as behavioral modifications, to deal with disease.

Results

Here, we demonstrate that activation of the immune system in honey bees (using bacterial lipopolysaccharides as a non-replicative pathogen) alters the social responses of healthy nestmates toward the treated individuals. Furthermore, treated individuals expressed significant differences in overall cuticular hydrocarbon profiles compared with controls. Finally, coating healthy individuals with extracts containing cuticular hydrocarbons of immunostimulated individuals significantly increased the agonistic responses of nestmates.

Conclusion

Since cuticular hydrocarbons play a critical role in nestmate recognition and other social interactions in a wide variety of insect species, modulation of such chemical profiles by the activation of the immune system could play a crucial role in the social regulation of pathogen dissemination within the colony.

Background

Defenses against pathogens and diseases have been remarkably well conserved across species, from invertebrates to vertebrates [1, 2]. In particular, the innate immune system, which responds to bacteria, fungi, viruses and other parasites, is very similar even at the molecular level in such diverse phyla as mammals, insects, and even plants [3, 4]. However, while molecular responses to pathogens have been the main focus of research, it has been demonstrated that behavioral responses could also serve a critical role in supporting the immune response to infection [5–7]. Research in mammals suggests that immune-stimulated individuals withdraw from their healthy social mates [8], which would represent an adaptive strategy to protect non-infected conspecifics [7]. However, it is still unclear whether alterations in social interactions represent a general and conserved mechanism implicated in the control of disease transmission within social groups.

Social insects, such as honey bees, ants and termites, are ideal models for studying mechanisms regulating disease transmission in social groups. Social insects provide an ideal environment for transmission of pathogens, since nests are typically environmentally controlled and there are hundreds if not thousands of potential hosts with frequent interactions, allowing for relatively easy dissemination [9–12]. Furthermore, based on modeling [13–15] and experimental studies [16, 17], the spread of disease within a colony is strongly dependent on social interactions between workers. Interestingly, though honey bees are the target of a large number of pathogens [18–21], they have substantially fewer genes associated with the innate immune response pathways compared with solitary insect species [22]. This suggests that honey bees may have developed alternative strategies to deal with pathogens.

Social insects have evolved multiple behavioral adaptations to avoid or combat parasites and pathogen infections. One suite of behaviors involves removing infected nestmates, or avoiding the source of infection. Zootermes angusticollis termites in experimentally controlled contact with spores (Metarhizium anisopliae) emit a vibratory signal that repels nestmates from the source of infection [23]. Coptotermes termites wall off nestmates infected by nematodes (Fujii 1975 cited in [24]). Honey bees reduce the impact of pathogenic bacteria, fungi and parasitic mites through hygienic behaviors, where workers identify and remove infected larvae from the healthy brood [25, 26]. Honey bees also can produce a 'social fever' where workers increase temperature in the brood patch to kill larval pathogens [27]. A second suite of behavioral responses to infection involves altering the behavior of the infected individual to reduce the transmission of the disease. Parasitized bumblebee workers stay outside overnight rather than returning to the nest and actively seek out colder temperatures [28], which delays the development of the parasite. Nosema-infected honey bee workers become foragers, no longer engage in in-hive tasks and brood care, and have less contact with the queen [29]. However, although these behavioral modifications may reduce the chances and/or speed of parasitic dissemination within the colony, they could also reflect some parasite manipulation of the host favoring its horizontal transmission to other colonies (for example, food-borne transmission [30]). A third behavioral modification involves altering social interactions between healthy and diseased nestmates. Increased contact between healthy and infected individuals can results in 'social' vaccination of the healthy individual, increasing its immunity [31]. Termites and ants in groups have decreased mortality from fungal infection compared with isolated individuals (for example, [31–33]), suggesting that social interactions such as grooming could have some important function. Virus-infected honey bee workers are the target of increased grooming [34]. However, increase social interactions could cause increase transmission as well [35]. In Formica polyctena ants there is increased social contact and allogrooming from untreated ants toward their immune-challenged conspecifics, resulting in reduced locomotion of the immune-challenged individual in cage studies [9]. Decreased locomotion of an infected individual could decrease the spread of pathogens in a colony [9, 31]. These studies suggest that nestmates can distinguish healthy from unhealthy nestmates, although in many cases, colony level studies need to be performed to determine if the behavioral modifications truly decrease disease spread. Furthermore mechanisms by which nestmates distinguish diseased and healthy workers are unknown.

Chemical communication plays a key role in regulating social interactions in social insects. Recent data confirm that cuticular hydrocarbons are involved in worker interactions and discrimination [36–38], and are generally implicated in the inter- and intra-specific recognition in many different species, including ants [39, 40], termites [41], wasps [42], and bees [43]. Social insects use cuticular hydrocarbons to distinguish between nestmates and non-nestmates, and to reject non-nestmate individuals [44, 45]. Hydrocarbon profiles are highly sensitive to genotype [46, 47], environment [38, 48], physiological state [49] and age in social insects (reviewed in [49]), suggesting that they could also be altered by immune processes. Indeed, there is some evidence for chemical communication of immunological status to nestmates. Parasitization by an ectoparasitic mite (Varroa destructor) significantly alters the cuticular hydrocarbon profile of emerging adult bees [50], but it is still unknown whether or not such changes in chemical profiles are correlated with altered nestmate responses to parasitized bees, and if these chemical changes are caused by the host's immune responses or factors produced by the mite.

Here we experimentally stimulated the immune system of honey bees, Apis mellifera, by injecting bees with a solution of bacterial coat proteins, that is, lipopolysaccharides (LPS), which has the advantage of stimulating an immune response in the host without spreading any infectious agent to the healthy nestmates. LPS is commonly used to stimulate immune responses in insects, and activates the JNK pathway of the innate immune system (see [51, 52]). Here, we confirmed immune system activation after LPS administration by measuring expression levels of an immune response gene (Defensin2) in the fat bodies of honey bee workers. We monitored social interactions between healthy honey bee workers and immune-challenged conspecifics. We analyzed the cuticular chemical profiles of immune-challenged and healthy individuals to determine if chemical communication can be modified by infection. Finally, we treated healthy worker bees with the cuticular extracts of control or LPS-injected workers and examined their interactions with untreated nestmates, to determine if the changes in the chemical profiles could indeed be responsible for the altered social interactions.

Results

Effects of LPS treatment on expression of Defensin2

We used quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) to measure Def2 expression levels in the workers' fat bodies to determine if LPS injection activated expression of immune response genes. Def2 expression was significantly affected by treatment (F(2,46) = 33.2; P < 0.001; Figure 1). There were also significant differences between colonies (F(2,46) = 10.07; P = 0.002) but no significant colony*treatment interaction (F(2,46) = 2.56; P = 0.08). Although saline-injection causes a significant increase in Def levels compared with the sham bees, LPS-injected bees had significantly higher levels of Def2 than both sham and saline-injected bees (Tukey's honestly significant differences (HSD) post-hoc tests, P < 0.05). Thus, while rupturing the cuticle by injection still stimulates the immune system, the effect of LPS is significantly larger.

Figure 1
figure 1

Lipopolysaccharide injection activates the immune system of honey bees. Relative expression levels of Defensin 2 (Def2), an immune response gene, are increased in the fat bodies of 7-day-old honey bee workers after saline- and lipopolysaccharide (LPS)-injection compared with untreated controls (sham). LPS-injection causes a significantly higher expression of Def2 than saline-injection. Relative expression levels of Def2 were monitored using quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction using fat bodies from individual bees. Data were collected from two independent trials, and each trial used bees from a different source colony (sham: N = 18; saline: N = 16; LPS: N = 18). Figure shows the effects of treatments on mean ± standard error of the mean quantitative expression of Def2 RNA (F(2,46) = 31.702; P < 0.001). Post-hoc tests:* P < 0.05; *** P < 0.001.

Effects of LPS treatment on locomotion and mortality

Lethality of our treatments was assessed 8 hours after treatment. After treatment, two out of 12 of the sham individuals, three out of 11 of the saline-injected individuals, and three out of 11 of the LPS-injected bees died. There was no significant difference between the three treatment groups (Chi2 = 0.485; df = 2; P > 0.78). No additional mortality was found 24 hours after treatment.

The behavioral effects of treatments (N = 8 for each treatment) were individually assessed on isolated bees placed in circular arenas for 5 minutes. Statistical analyses revealed no significant effects of treatments on general activity (that is, total time spent self-grooming: sham: 18.5 ± 17.5; saline-injected: 9 ± 17.6; LPS-injected: 9.5 ± 37.7 seconds) or locomotion (that is, number of lines crossed: sham: 127 ± 39; saline-injected: 137 ± 22; LPS-injected: 145 ± 37) (H(2)<0.98; P > 0.61).

Effects of LPS treatment on nestmates' social interactions

To monitor social interactions, we used a modified nestmate recognition assay in which a treated animal is placed back in a cage with its nestmates and social interactions are monitored. This assay has been used regularly in bees to examine nestmate recognition, and reliably reflects similar interactions in field colonies [44, 53]. As above, there was no effect on locomotion of treated individuals during the social interaction assay (H(2) = 1.74; P = 0.41; data not shown). Concerning social interactions, agonistic and non-agonistic contacts were analyzed separately. Indeed, the calculation of a global 'aggression index' as it is commonly used in behavioral studies in insects would have been statistically irrelevant since agonistic behaviors (that is, opening mandibles, biting, gaster flexion, stinging) were virtually absent from our observations. Indeed, none of the 12 sham-treated, and only one out of 11 saline-injected and two out of 12 LPS-injected bees were attacked. Moreover, these attacks included only opening of mandibles and biting. Thus, although there was a slight trend for increased agonistic behavior toward LPS-injected bees, there was no overall significant difference in the proportion of agonistic interactions between the three groups (Chi2 = 2.132; df = 2; P > 0.344). However, there were significant differences in the frequency of non-agonistic contacts (that is, antennating and allogrooming) of the healthy nestmates with individuals between our three experimental groups (H(2) = 11.68; P < 0.002, Figure 2). Post-hoc comparisons revealed that LPS-injected subjects received significantly more non-agonistic social contacts than sham or saline-injected workers (Dunn's post-hoc tests, P < 0.01 and P < 0.05 respectively; Figure 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Immunostimulated bees receive increased social interactions from untreated nestmates. Bees were reared in cages, 10/cage until they were 7 days old. Individual bees were removed from cages and handled and treated with carbon dioxide (sham), injected with saline, or injected with lipopolysaccharides (LPS). After 4 hours, treated bees were returned to their original cage and non-agonistic social interactions (antennating, allogrooming) with untreated nestmates were monitored during a 5-minute period. Data were collected into two independent colonies (12 sham bees; 11 saline-injected bees; 12 LPS-injected bees). Figure shows effects of treatment on median ± the semi interquartile range frequency of social interactions (H(2) = 11.68; P < 0.002). Post-hoc tests: * P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01.

Overall, these results show that LPS increased general social activities in healthy nestmates towards the immune-challenged insects, but did not induce a specific agonistic outcome.

Effects of LPS treatment on cuticular hydrocarbon profiles

Non-polar compounds were extracted from insects' whole bodies using pentane, and chemical profiles of cuticular extracts were analyzed using gas chromatography (GC). In our experimental conditions, all the chemical compounds we identified by mass spectrometry (MS) analysis were hydrocarbons: alkanes, alkenes, alkynes, and methyl-alkanes. The comparison of the cuticular profiles of sham, saline or LPS-injected workers 4 hours after injection revealed significant statistical differences in the overall chemical profiles of samples collected from colony SDI8 (F(14,46) = 5.69, P < 10-4, Figure 3a and Table 1). Mahalanobis chemical distances between both the sham/saline-injected and sham/LPS-injected bees were significantly different (MD = 5.02; P < 0.02 and MD = 12.07; P < 0.0001, respectively). Moreover, Mahalanobis chemical distance between LPS-injected bees and saline-injected bees was also significantly different (MD = 13.2; P < 0.0001). Table 1 summarizes the relative proportion of each chemical compound on the workers' cuticles; out of 32 compounds analyzed, significant changes in relative proportion were only found for seven compounds. Furthermore, no new compounds were found in the LPS-treated sample. Thus immunostimulation is associated with variation in the relative proportion of existing compounds.

Figure 3
figure 3

Immunostimulated bees display significantly different cuticular hydrocarbon chemical profiles. Discriminant analysis was performed on the cuticular chemical profiles of sham, saline- and lipopolysaccharide-injected honey bee workers based on their relative proportions of chemical compounds (a, colony SDI8, F(14,46) = 5.69, P < 0.0001). Data were obtained from gas chromatography analysis of cuticular washes. Similar results were obtained for bees derived from a different colony in a second independent trial (b, colony SDI11, (F(26,10) = 6.74; P = 0.0016).

Table 1 Effect of immuno-stimulation on cuticular hydrocarbon profiles

Similar results for the comparison of the cuticular profiles of sham, saline and LPS-injected bees were obtained with bees from a second colony (colony SDI11, F(26,10) = 6.74; P = 0.0016, Figure 3b and Additional file 1. Mahalanobis chemical distances between both the sham/saline-injected and sham/LPS-injected bees were significantly different (MD = 35.6; P < 0.001 and MD = 182.4; P < 0.0001, respectively). Moreover, Mahalanobis chemical distance between LPS-injected bees and saline-injected bees was also significantly different (MD = 63.8; P < 0.0001). However, different chemicals were found to show significant variations in relative proportion compared with colony SDI8 (Table 1 and Additional file 1). Thus, immunostimulation causes colony-specific variation in hydrocarbon cuticular proportions.

Since alkenes seem to play an important role in nestmate recognition in bees [37], we assessed the effect of treatments on the relative proportions of alkenes. Treatment had a significant effect on the relative proportion of the different alkenes between workers from colony SDI8 (F(12,50) = 3.6; P = 0.0007). Mahalanobis chemical distances between sham/saline-injected, sham/LPS-injected, and saline-injected/LPS-injected bees were significantly different (MD = 4.06; P < 0.02, MD = 6.51; P < 0.001, and MD = 4.03; P = 0.017, respectively). Similar results were obtained with colony SDI11 (F(10,26) = 2.37; P = 0.037). However, Mahalanobis chemical distances between sham/saline-injected, sham/LPS-injected bees were not significantly different (MD = 1.59; P > 0.05 and MD = 0.98; P > 0.05, respectively) but saline-injected/LPS-injected were significantly different (MD = 2.25; P = 0.03).

Effect of immunostimulated nestmate cuticular extract on nestmate interactions

The following study was performed to determine if the changes in chemical profiles associated with immunostimulation are indeed sufficient to alter nestmate social interactions. Bees from a single colony were reared in cages as before. Sham or LPS-injected individuals were kept in isolation for 4 hours (as before) and then collected. Cuticular extracts were collected from these individuals and pooled according to treatment. A naïve bee from a new cage was then coated with 5 μl of extract (1 worker equivalent) from the cuticles of sham or LPS-injected workers. Coated bees were then placed back in their original cages, and social interactions were monitored as before. An 'aggression index' [38] was then calculated from the frequency of social behaviors observed. Indeed, the changes induced by coating generated a suitable amount of agonistic behaviors to allow a valid calculation of this behavioral index. Analyses revealed that coatings significantly altered social interactions in worker bees. The presence of a bee coated with cuticular extracts from LPS-injected workers induced significantly more agonistic behaviors (that is, a significantly higher aggression index) in nestmates, compared with a bee coated with cuticular extracts from sham bees (Mann and Whitney U = 10; P = 0.0001, Figure 4).

Figure 4
figure 4

Extracts of immunostimulated workers increases nestmate social interactions. Bees were reared in cages, 15/cage until they were 7 days old. One naïve bee was removed from each cage and coated with 5 μl of extracts from sham or lipopolysaccharide (LPS)-treated bees (one bee equivalent). After the bees were coated, each one was returned to her original cage and social interactions with untreated nestmates were monitored during a 5-minute period. Data shown represent median ± the semi interquartile range values of the aggression index, calculated from measured social behaviors [38]. Treatment groups consisted of 12 bees coated with cuticular extracts of sham bees, and 12 bees coated with cuticular extracts from LPS-treated workers. 'LPS coating' induced significantly more interactions (agonistic) compared with the control (U = 10; P = 0.0001).

Discussion

Using bacterial endotoxins (that is, LPS), we activated the innate immune system of individual insects, and monitored the responses of healthy nestmates. Although both LPS- and saline-injection caused an increase in expression of an immune response gene (as seen in previous studies [54]), this increase was substantially and significantly higher for the LPS-injected individuals, and only LPS-injection induced significant changes in social interactions between treated animals and their healthy conspecifics. The evaluation of the lethality and behavioral effects of LPS on treated honey bees confirm that the social changes observed were not the result of behavioral alterations in treated individuals. Furthermore, these differences in social interactions correlated with changes in cuticular hydrocarbon profiles induced by injection; again, these changes were greater for the LPS- versus saline-treated bees. Finally, we found that the coating of naïve bees with cuticular extracts from LPS-injected bees induced significant changes in social behaviors of healthy nestmates compared with controls. In sum, these results suggest that immune system activation can have significant effects on honey bee cuticular chemical profiles, which in turn results in changes in social interactions with nestmates.

Both injection with LPS and coating with extracts of an LPS-injected individual stimulated changes in social responses of healthy nestmates towards the treated individual. However, treatment with LPS elicited only increases in non-agonistic interactions (antennating, grooming) while coating with extract from LPS-injected individuals stimulated agonistic interactions (biting, stinging). Indeed, the coating of a social insect with exogenous chemicals is known to induce aggression in nestmates since this induces global changes in the chemical profile of the coated individual, although coating ants with the cuticular hydrocarbons of their nestmates does not elicit an aggressive response [55]. These results suggest that if the proportions of chemicals are changed, social interactions are modified. It is possible in our experiments that coating individuals with extracts caused additional changes to the chemical profiles that elicited a stronger behavioral response from the healthy nestmates. Regardless, both experiments confirm that LPS (injected or the associated changes in chemical profiles) alters social responses of healthy nestmates compared with controls.

The altered social interactions could benefit the individual, the nestmates or the pathogen. Grooming appears to play some crucial role in individual resistance to pathogens in social insects. For both termites and ants, individuals infected with a pathogenic fungus that were placed in groups are less likely to die than those reared in isolation [15, 32, 33]. Grooming was not specifically observed in either of these studies, although Hughes et al [33] found a decreased amount of fungus on the cuticle over time. However, this physical removal was observed in both isolated and group-reared termites. Virus-infected bees were found to be subjected to increased grooming [34]. Thus, ants, termites, and honey bees may all increase grooming of nestmates in response to a variety of pathogens. Furthermore, while grooming is clearly beneficial to the infected individual, it may also benefit the group by resulting in a 'social vaccination' of the naïve nestmate. Indeed, rearing naïve termites with termites previously exposed to pathogens can improve the subsequent resistance of the naïve termites to a challenge with a fungal pathogen [31]. This anticipatory immuno-activation could be induced by grooming, either through contact with small amounts of pathogen fragments, or with immuno-active molecules produced by the infected host and thus stimulating immune defenses of naïve conspecifics (reviewed in [9]). However, social interactions can obviously also increase the spread of pathogen. Further studies using full colonies and active pathogens will be necessary to determine the effects of behavioral modifications towards unhealthy nestmates on pathogen transmission.

Our studies suggest that immunostimulation causes changes in cuticular hydrocarbon patterns that result in altered social interactions. Upon immune stimulation, fat bodies and hematocytes secrete antimicrobial peptides and enzymes into the hemolymph [56]. As noted by Schal et al [57], an active association exists between hemolymph and the cuticle since compounds from the hemolymph are readily transported to the outer epicuticular surface. Indeed, hydrocarbons are produced by oenocytes and are transported through a hemolymph pathway to both external and internal tissues, including the epicuticle, fat bodies, and ovaries [58]. Our studies also suggest that the effects of immunostimulation do not cause addition of novel cuticular hydrocarbon compounds, and variation in these compounds appear colony specific. Thus, immunostimulation may alter existing biosynthesis or transport pathways, and thereby shift cuticular chemical profiles. Previous studies demonstrated that alkenes are important for nestmate recognition [37], and in our studies there were significant differences in the relative proportion of alkenes between saline-injected and LPS-injected bees from two different colonies. However, different specific alkenes were modulated by treatment in the two colonies, suggesting that immune stimulation does not produce a specific 'disease' signal pattern, but rather alters overall cuticular hydrocarbon patterns in a non-specific way that may differ for bees derived from different genotypic or environmental backgrounds. Alterations of cuticular hydrocarbons and pheromones by pathogens remain largely unexplored phenomena, but it could be relatively general. Indeed, parasitization by an ectoparasite mite (Varroa destructor) during larvae development significantly affects the relative proportion of cuticular hydrocarbons of emerging adult bees [50], while in mealworm beetles, male immunocompetence and parasite load appears to be communicated to females via changes in sex pheromones [59, 60].

Conclusion

Here, we provide clear evidence of altered behaviors in healthy individuals toward immuno-stimulated conspecifics in honey bees. Previous studies on mammalian species such as mice or rats demonstrated a social disinterest or withdrawal of immune-stimulated individuals [8], and this has been argued to represent an adaptive strategy to protect healthy conspecifics [7]. In our studies, immune stimulation leads to the increased tendency of healthy individuals to enter into contact with their infected nestmate. This could lead to an increase in transmission of the pathogen, decreased transmission by slowing the movement of infected individuals, physically reduce the pathogen or parasite load on the infected individual, or allow 'social vaccination' of the healthy nestmates. Further studies will be necessary to determine the effect of the observed behavioral modification on pathogen transmission. Our study also suggests that immunostimulated individuals signal their status to healthy nestmates via changes in cuticular hydrocarbon profiles. Since chemical communication regulates social interactions in a wide variety of species, including insects and vertebrates, modulation of chemical cues by the immune system may play an important and general role in the regulation of social behaviors.

Methods

General bee rearing

Honey bee colonies were maintained according to standard commercial procedures at North Carolina State University's Honey Bee Research Facility. Bees were derived from colonies headed by queens instrumentally inseminated with semen from a single, different drone (Apis mellifera carnica, from Glenn Apiaries, CA, US [61]); thus, due to the haplodiploidy of honey bees, the coefficient of relatedness among the offspring in each colony was 0.75. Since cuticular hydrocarbon profiles and nestmate interactions are very sensitive to genotypic variation [46, 47], using highly related bees within individual trials was important for our studies. Four colonies were used for these studies: SDI8, SDI11, SDI72, and SDI73. The specific source colonies used for the different experiments are noted in each experimental section.

To provide bees of known age, frames containing late-stage pupae were removed and placed in a 33°C incubator for adult bees to emerge. One-day-old workers were placed in 10-cm diameter Petri dishes (15 bees/dish), and dishes were kept in a humidity (40% relative humidity) and temperature (33°C)-controlled environmental chamber under red light. Bees were fed a 50% sucrose/water solution, which was changed every 2 days. Bees were also treated with 0.1 queen bee equivalents of queen mandibular pheromone (QMP) (Pherotech, Canada). Every day, 10 μl of QMP were placed on a microscope slide and allowed to evaporate before being placed in the cage. This amount of QMP mimics a live queen in assays of worker behavior and physiology [62–64], and thus should help simulate normal rearing conditions. When the honeybee workers were 7 days old, individual bees were subjected to the following treatments: sham (handled and anesthetized with carbon dioxide, but not injected), saline injected (anesthetized with carbon dioxide and injected with a physiological solution), LPS injected (anesthetized with carbon dioxide and injected with a solution of LPS, see below for details). Injected bees were maintained individually in separate Plexiglas cages (10 × 10 × 7 cm3) and fed 50% sucrose ad libitum for 4 hours after injection, before being collected or used in behavioral assays. For the behavioral assays, the injected bees were subjected to a modified nestmate recognition assay [65]; these assays were performed with colony SDI11, colony SDI72, and colony SDI73. For the cuticular hydrocarbon analysis, injected bees were collected on to dry ice 4 hours after treatment; these assays were performed with bees from colony SDI11 and colony SDI8 (in two separate trials). For the gene expression analysis, injected bees were collected on to dry ice 4 hours after treatment; these assays were performed with bees from colony SDI11 and colony SDI8 in two separate trials. See below for detailed methods.

Bacterial LPS solution

LPS is the major outer surface membrane component present in almost all Gram-negative bacteria and acts as an extremely strong stimulator of innate or natural immunity in diverse eukaryotic species ranging from insects to humans [66, 67]. Based on previous trials and pilot studies, LPS-treatment consisted of a 4 μl injection of 500 ng/μl LPS (from Escherichia coli serotype 055:B5, Sigma) diluted in physiological (saline) solution (130 mM NaCl, 6 mM KCl, 4 mM MgCl2, 5 mM CaCl2, 160 mM sucrose, 25 mM glucose, 10 mM 4-(2-hydroxyethyl)-1-piperazineethanesulfonic acid in distilled water, pH 6.7, 500 mOsmol). Injections were performed into the abdominal cavity through tergites with a nano-injector equipped with a glass needle (Schley Compact Model II Instrument; Honey Bee Insemination Services, Davis, CA, US), using a binocular microscope for guidance (Leica MZ6 stereomicroscope).

Expression level of Defensin2 (Def2) in abdominal fat body

We next tested if LPS treatment activated the expression of immune response genes in the fat bodies of 7-day-old worker bees, 4 hours after injection. Previous studies demonstrated that RNA expression of the antimicrobial peptide Defensin was significantly increased in honey bee workers upon injection with E. coli (reference [54]; and references therein). In our studies, we focused on Defensin2 since this gene was more highly up-regulated in microbe-injected honey bee larvae than Defensin1 [68]. Abdomens were dissected under RNAlater (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, US) to remove intestines, and the remaining cuticles with the associated fat bodies were collected. Total RNA was isolated from the fat bodies using an RNeasy RNA extraction kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA, US), yielding 0.6–1.5 μg/individual bee. cDNA was synthesized from 150 ng RNA using Arrayscript reverse transcriptase (Ambion, Foster City, CA, US). Expression levels of Defensin2 were measured using quantitative RT-PCR (qRT-PCR) with an ABI Prism 7900 sequence detector and the SYBR green detection method (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, US). eIF3-S8, a housekeeping gene that did not vary in expression levels in previous bee microarray studies [62, 69] was used as a loading control. For each sample, triplicate qRT-PCR reactions were performed and averaged. A standard curve was generated for each primer using dilutions of genomic DNA to calculate the relative quantities of RNA levels for each sample. A dissociation curve and negative control (cDNA reaction without RT-enzyme) were used to ensure primer specificity and lack of contamination. Primers were designed using PrimerExpress software (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, US). The sequences for the primers (5' to 3') used are as follows:

Def2-F: CAGAATTGATGGATTCCAACGA3;

Def2-R: CGCACGTTACCCTTCGATGT;

eIFS8-F: TGAGTGTCTGCTATGGATTGCAA;

eIFS8-R: TCGCGGCTCGTGGTAAA;

We evaluated the fat body gene expression levels of seven sham bees, seven saline-injected, and seven LPS-injected bees from colony SDI11, and 11 sham bees, nine saline-injected and 11 LPS-injected bees from colony SDI8. For each individual fat body sample, the quantities of Def2 and eIF3-S8 were calculated based on the standard curve. The ratio of the relative quantity of Def2 RNA levels (based on the standard curve) to that of the control gene (eIF3-S8) was calculated for each sample.

Behavioral assessment of the deleterious effects of treatment

To determine whether LPS induced any deleterious effects in treated insects, we conducted a preliminary study in 7-day-old honey bees (N = 8 per treatment, from colony SDI8). Lethality and behavioral alterations (that is, changes in grooming and mobility) were individually monitored 8 hours after treatment. To assess mobility, a cross was drawn under the circular arena and the number of lines crossed during 5 minutes was used as a locomotion index. We also recorded lethality after the nestmate interaction assays (see below).

Behavioral effects of treatments in nestmate interactions

To assess the effect of immune stimulation on nestmate interactions, an individual worker bee was picked up from the Petri dish where she was raised (10 bees/dish were used for these experiments), marked with a dot of enamel paint on the thorax and subjected to one of the three treatments: sham, saline-injected or LPS-injected. After the injection, each individual bee was isolated in a Plexiglas cage (10 × 10 × 7 cm3) for 4 hours. Then, the worker was reintroduced to the original Petri dish. Studies were conducted blind to the treatment group. Each cage was used only once.

Beginning 30 seconds after the reintroduction, the number and type of social behaviors of the non-treated bees toward the treated bee were recorded through a scanning method, every 30 seconds for 5 minutes. Two types of behavior were observed: non-agonistic social contacts (that is, antennal contacts, allogrooming) and agonistic interactions (that is, mandibular openings, bites, gaster flexions, stinging). Moreover, as it is commonly used in behavioral studies, an index of locomotor activity was used to assess the effects of treatments on the subjects' mobility. This index was obtained as described above. Mortality of the injected individual was recorded as well.

Behavioral assays involved bees from three different colonies. Groups consisted of 12 sham (five from colony SDI11, four from colony SDI72, and three from colony SDI73), 11 saline-injected (five from colony SDI11, four from colony SDI72, and three from colony SDI73), and 12 LPS-injected (five from colony SDI11, four from colony SDI72, and two from colony SDI73) subjects.

Cuticular hydrocarbons analysis

We next tested if LPS-treatment altered the cuticular hydrocarbon profiles. Seven-day-old bees were collected 4 hours after the injection, as described for the behavioral assays. Each worker bee was submerged in 1 ml in pentane for 10 minutes to extract non-polar cuticular compounds. All the samples were maintained in the freezer at -20°C. After solvent evaporation, the samples were diluted in 50 μl of pentane with C16 as an internal standard (0.4 μg/μl).

GC was used to measure the relative proportions of the cuticular hydrocarbons in all of the samples. The analysis was performed on a HP 5890 equipped with capillary column (30 m × 0.25 mm inner diameter, 0.5 μm film thickness) DB-5 column (J&W Scientific, Folsom, CA, US) in splitless mode. Helium was used as the carrier gas at a head pressure of 18 psi (flow rate, 1.3 ml/minute). The oven temperature was held at 150°C for 2 minutes, increased to 250°C at 15°C/minute, increased to 300°C at 5°C/minute, and held for 20 minute. Injector and flame ionization detector temperatures were set at 300°C and 300°C, respectively.

For identification of the compounds present in the hydrocarbon profiles, one sample was subjected to GC/MS. A 2 μl portion was analyzed by splitless capillary GC/MS, using a Hewlett-Packard 6890 and a model 5973A msd with an electron impact ion source, HP-5ms (5% diphenyl-95% dimethylsiloxane) capillary column, 30 m × 0.25 mm inner diameter, 0.25 μm film thickness. Here, the oven temperature was held at 80°C for 2 minutes, increased to 200°C at 15°C/minute, increased to 300°C at 5°C/minute, and held for 15 minutes.

Under our extraction method, only hydrocarbons were apparent in the extract. We evaluated the cuticular chemical profiles of 10 sham (non-injected bees), 11 saline-injected and 12 LPS-injected bees from colony SDI8 and in a second trial compared bees (N = 5 for sham, N = 9 for saline-injected, and N = 7 for LPS-injected bees) from colony SDI11.

Effect of immuno-stimulated nestmate cuticular extract on social interactions between nestmates

To determine if the changes in cuticular chemical profile associated with immunostimulation was indeed associated with the observed behavioral assays, we tested the effects of coating naïve bees with cuticular extracts from LPS-injected bees (or sham bees for controls). Bees from a single colony were reared in Petri dishes as above (SDI33). Two Petri dishes were set up a day before the dishes used for the behavioral assay. Bees from these cages were treated with either carbon dioxide (sham) or injected with LPS, as above, and were collected 4 hours after treatment. The cuticular extraction was performed as previously described; all the extracts for each treatment were pooled.

For the behavioral assay, all bees were marked on their thorax with a dot of enamel paint, except for the bee used for the behavioral assay. The test bee was removed from the cage and coated with 5 μl of a solution (sham cuticular extract or LPS-injected cuticular extract), which represent one bee equivalent. After full evaporation of the solvent, the bee was reintroduced into her original cage and observed every 15 seconds for 5 minutes. Non-agonistic social contacts (that is, antennal contacts, allogrooming) and agonistic interactions (that is, mandible openings, bites, gaster flexions, stinging) were observed and compiled to calculate an 'aggression index' [38]. Indeed, as stated earlier, the social changes induced by coating generated a suitable amount of agonistic behaviors for a valid calculation of this behavioral index, which represents a valuable tool, commonly used in studies of insect behavior.

The entire experiment was repeated in a second trial, using bees from colony SDI33. Treatment groups consisted of 12 bees coated with sham extracts and 12 bees coated with extracts from LPS-treated bees.

Statistics

For the gene expression studies, the Def2/eIF3-S8 ratios for each individual were normalized to the average value for the sham-treated group for each colony, and a parametric analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with colony, treatment and colony*treatment interaction as variables, followed by post-hoc pairwise comparisons using Tukey's HSD tests. For the behavioral assays, owing to sample size and behavioral heterogeneity, non-parametric procedures were used to assess significant variations in our data [70]. Lethality of treatments was analyzed with a Chi2 test. Comparisons of social interactions between sham, saline-injected, and LPS-injected bees were performed using the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA on ranks for global comparison, followed by Dunn's analyses for multiple comparisons. For assessing cuticular hydrocarbon profile similarity, a stepwise discriminant analysis was employed using Statistica 6.0. (StatSoft® Inc.) Prior to analysis, each peak area was standardized according to Reyment (1989) [71]. Only peaks that were reliably and reproducibly quantifiable were used; peaks that were consistently below 0.1% of the total quantity were omitted. The chemical distance between each group was estimated as the Mahalanobis distance obtained from the discriminant analysis. GC analysis of cuticular hydrocarbons was conducted as in [38]. The effect of the treatment (sham, saline-injected, and LPS-injected) on the compound proportions was evaluated with a non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA on ranks for global comparison.

Abbreviations

ANOVA:

analysis of variance

GC:

gas chromatography

HSD:

honestly significant differences

LPS:

lipopolysaccharides

MS:

mass spectrometry

QMP:

queen mandibular pheromone

RT-PCR:

real-time polymerase chain reaction.

References

  1. Borregard N, Elsbach P, Ganz T, Garred P, Svejgaard A: Innate immunity from plants to humans. Immunol Today. 2000, 21: 68-70. 10.1016/S0167-5699(99)01570-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Aubert A: Invertebrates studies and the evolution of comparative psychoneuroimmunology. Brain Behav Immun. 2007, 21: 290-291. 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.01.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Magor BG, Magor KE: Evolution of effectors and receptors of innate immunity. Dev Comp Immunol. 2001, 25: 651-682. 10.1016/S0145-305X(01)00029-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Hoffmann JA: The immune response of Drosophila. Nature. 2003, 426: 33-38. 10.1038/nature02021.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Aubert A: Sickness and behaviour in animals: a motivational perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999, 23: 1029-1036. 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00034-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Aubert A, Renault J: Cytokines and immune-related behaviors. Neuroimmune Biology. Cytokines and the Brain. Edited by: Korneva E, Phelps C. 2008, Oxford: Elsevier Science, 6:

    Google Scholar 

  7. Hart BL: Behavioral adaptations to pathogens and parasites: five strategies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1988, 14: 273-294. 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80038-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dantzer R: Innate immunity at the forefront of psychoneuroimmunology. Brain Behav Immun. 2004, 18: 1-6. 10.1016/j.bbi.2003.09.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Aubert A, Richard FJ: Social management of LPS-induced inflammation in Formica polyctena ants. Brain Behav Immun. 2008, 22: 833-837. 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.01.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Cremer S, Armitage SAO, Schmid-Hempel P: Social immunity. Curr Biol. 2007, 17: R693-R702. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Sherman PW, Seeley T, Reeve HK: Parasites, pathogens, and polyandry in social Hymenoptera. Am Nat. 1988, 131: 602-610. 10.1086/284809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Sherman PW, Seeley T, Reeve HK: Parasites, pathogens, and polyandry in honey bees. Am Nat. 1998, 151: 392-396. 10.1086/286127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Naug D, Camazine S: The role of colony organization on pathogen transmission in social insects. J Theor Biol. 2002, 215: 427-439. 10.1006/jtbi.2001.2524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Pie MR, Rosengaus RB, Traniello JF: Nest architecture, activity pattern, worker density and the dynamics of disease transmission in social insects. J Theor Biol. 2004, 226: 45-51. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2003.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fefferman NH, Traniello JF, Rosengaus RB, Calleri DV: Disease prevention and resistance in social insects: modeling the survival consequences of immunity, hygienic behavior, and colony organization. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2007, 61: 565-577. 10.1007/s00265-006-0285-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Naug D, Smith B: Experimentally induced change in infectious period affects transmission dynamics in a social group. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2007, 274: 61-65. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Schmid-Hempel P, Schmid-Hempel R: Transmission of a pathogen in Bombus terrestris, with a note on division of labour in social insects. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993, 33: 319-327. 10.1007/BF00172930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Allen M, Ball B: The incidence and world distribution of the honey bee viruses. Bee World. 1996, 77: 141-162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ball BV, Allen MF: The prevalence of pathogens in honey bee colonies infested with the parasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni. Ann Appl Biol. 1988, 113: 237-244. 10.1111/j.1744-7348.1988.tb03300.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Martin SJ: The role of Varroa and viral pathogens in the collapse of honey bee colonies: a modelling approach. J Appl Ecol. 2001, 38: 1082-1093. 10.1046/j.1365-2664.2001.00662.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Morse RA, Nowogrodski R: Honey Bee Pests, Predators and Diseases. 1990, Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, Comstock Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  22. Evans JD, Aronstein K, Chen YP, Hetru C, Imler JL, Jiang H, Kanost M, Thompson GJ, Zou Z, Hultmark D: Immune pathways and defense mechanisms in honey bees Apis mellifera. Insect Mol Biol. 2006, 15: 645-656. 10.1111/j.1365-2583.2006.00682.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Rosengaus RB, Jordan C, Lefebvre ML, Traniello JFA: Pathogen alarm behavior in a termite: a new form of communication in social insect. Naturwissenschaften. 1999, 86: 544-548. 10.1007/s001140050672.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Little AEF, Murakami T, Mueller UG, Currie CR: Defending against parasites: fungus-growing ants combine specialized behaviours and microbial symbionts to protect their fungus gardens. Biol Lett. 2006, 2: 12-16. 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0371.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Spivak M, Reuter GS: Resistance to American foulbrood disease by honey bee colonies, Apis mellifera, bred for hygienic behavior. Apidologie. 2001, 32: 555-565. 10.1051/apido:2001103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Ibrahim A, Spivak M: The relationship between hygienic behavior and suppression of mite reproduction as honey bee (Apis mellifera) mechanisms of resistance to Varroa destructor. Apidologie. 2006, 37: 31-40. 10.1051/apido:2005052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Starks PT, Blackie CA, Seeley T: Fever in honeybee colonies. Naturwissenschaften. 2000, 87: 229-231. 10.1007/s001140050709.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Müller CB, Schmid-Hempel P: Exploitation of cold temperature as defence against parasitoids in bumble bees. Nature. 1993, 363: 65-67. 10.1038/363065a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Wang DI, Moeller FE: The division of labor and queen attendance behavior of Nosema-infected worker honey bees. J Econ Entomol. 1970, 63: 1539-1542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Chen Y, Evans J, Feldlaufer M: Horizontal and vertical transmission of viruses in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. J Invertebr Pathol. 2006, 92: 152-159. 10.1016/j.jip.2006.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Traniello JFA, Rosengaus RB, Savoie K: The development of immunity in a social evidence for the group facilitation of disease resistance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002, 99: 6838-6842. 10.1073/pnas.102176599.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Calleri DV, McGrail Reid E, Rosengaus RB, Vargo EL, Traniello JFA: Inbreeding and disease resistance in a social insect: effects of heterozygosity on immunocompetence in the termite Zootermopsis angusticollis. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2006, 273: 2633-2640. 10.1098/rspb.2006.3622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Hughes WOH, Eilenberg J, Boomsma JJ: Trade-offs in group living: transmission and disease resistance in leaf-cutting ants. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2002, 269: 1811-1819. 10.1098/rspb.2002.2113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Waddington KD, Rothenbuhler WC: Behavior associated with hairless-black syndrome of adult honey bees. J Apic Res. 1976, 15: 35-41.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Bailey L, Ball BV: Honey Bee Pathology. 1991, London: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  36. Châline N, Sandoz JC, Martin SJ, Ratnieks FLW, Jones GR: Learning and discrimination of individual cuticular hydrocarbons by honey bees (Apis mellifera). Chem Senses. 2005, 30: 327-335. 10.1093/chemse/bji027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Dani FR, Jones GR, Corsi S, Beard R, Pradella D, Turillazzi S: Nestmate recognition cues in the honey bee: differential importance of cuticular alkanes and alkenes. Chem Senses. 2005, 30: 477-489. 10.1093/chemse/bji040.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Richard FJ, Hefetz A, Christides JP, Errard C: Food influence on colonial recognition and chemical signature between nestmates in the fungus-growing ant Acromyrmex subterraneus. Chemoecology. 2004, 14: 9-16. 10.1007/s00049-003-0251-3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Richard FJ, Poulsen M, Drijfhout F, Jones GR, Boomsma JJ: Specificity in chemical profiles of workers, brood and mutualistic fungi in Atta, Acromyrmex, and Sericomyrmex fungus-growing ants. J Chem Ecol. 2007, 33: 2281-2292. 10.1007/s10886-007-9385-z.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Meer Vander RK, Morel L: Nestmate recognition in ants. Pheromone Communication in Social Insects. Edited by: Vander Meer RK, Breed M, Espelie KE, Winston M. 1998, Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 79-103.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Singer TL: Roles of hydrocarbons in the recognition systems of insects. Am Zool. 1998, 38: 394-405.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Lorenzi MC, Bagneres AG, Clément JL, Turillazzi S: Polistes biglumis bimaculatus epiculticular hydrocarbons and nestmate recognition (Hymenoptera Vespidae). Insectes Soc. 1997, 44: 123-138. 10.1007/s000400050035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Smith BH, Breed MD: The chemical basis for nestmate recognition and mate discrimination in insects. Chemical Ecology of Insects. Edited by: Cardé RT, Bell WJ. 1995, New York: Chapman & Hall, 2: 287-317.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  44. Breed MD: Nestmate recognition in honey bees. Anim Behav. 1983, 31: 86-91. 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80176-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Wilson EO: The Insect Societies. 1971, Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press

    Google Scholar 

  46. Breed MD, Butler L, Stiller TM: Kin discrimination by worker honeybees in genetically mixed groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1985, 82: 3058-3061. 10.1073/pnas.82.9.3058.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Getz WM, Smith K: Honey bee kin recognition: learning self and nestmate phenotypes. Anim Behav. 1986, 34: 1617-1626. 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80250-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Liang D, Silverman J: 'You are what you eat': diet modifies cuticular hydrocarbons and nestmate recognition in the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile. Naturwissenschaften. 2000, 87: 412-416. 10.1007/s001140050752.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Howard RW, Blomquist GJ: Ecological, behavioral, and biochemical aspects of insect hydrocarbons. Annu Rev Entomol. 2005, 50: 371-393. 10.1146/annurev.ento.50.071803.130359.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Salvy M, Martin C, Bagnères AG, Provost E, Roux M, Le Conte Y, Clement JL: Modifications of the cuticular hydrocarbon profile of Apis mellifera worker bees in the presence of the ectoparasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni in brood cells. Parasitology. 2001, 122: 145-159. 10.1017/S0031182001007181.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Boutros M, Agaisse H, Perrimon N: Sequential activation of signalling pathways during innate immune responses in Drosophila. Dev Cell. 2002, 3: 711-722. 10.1016/S1534-5807(02)00325-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Sluss HK, Han Z, Barrett T, Davis RJ, Ip YT: A JNK signal transduction pathway that mediates morphogenesis and an immune response in Drosophila. Genes Dev. 1996, 10: 2745-2758. 10.1101/gad.10.21.2745.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Robinson GE: Modulation of alarm pheromone perception in the honey bee: evidence for division of labor based on hormonally modulated response thresholds. J Comp Physiol. 1987, 160: 613-619. 10.1007/BF00611934.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Yang X, Cox-Foster D: Impact of an ectoparasite on the immunity and pathology of an invertebrate: evidence for host immunosuppression and viral amplification. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005, 102: 7470-7475. 10.1073/pnas.0501860102.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Brandt M, Tsutsui ND: The role of cuticular hydrocarbons as chemical cues for nestmate recognition in the invasive Argentine ant (Linepithema humile). Insectes Soc. 2007, 54: 363-373. 10.1007/s00040-007-0954-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Kim T, Kim Y-J: Overview of innate immunity in Drosophila. J Biochem Mol Biol. 2005, 38 (2): 121-127.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Schal C, Sevala V, Card RT: Novel and highly specific transport of a volatile sex pheromone by hemolymph lipophorin in moths. Naturwissenschaften. 1998, 85: 339-342. 10.1007/s001140050511.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Fan Y, Zurek L, Dykstra MJ, Schal C: Hydrocarbon synthesis by enzymatically dissociated oenocytes of the abdominal integument of the German cockroach, Blatella germanica. Naturwissenschaften. 2003, 90: 121-126.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Rantala MJ, Jokinen I, Kortet R, Vainikka A, Suhonen J: Do pheromones reveal male immunocompetence?. Proc Biol Sci. 2002, 269: 1681-1685. 10.1098/rspb.2002.2056.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Worden BD, Parker PG, Pappas PW: Parasites reduce attractiveness and reproductive success in male grain beetles. Anim Behav. 2000, 59: 543-550. 10.1006/anbe.1999.1368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Laidlaw HHJ: Instrumental Insemination of Honey Bee Queens: Its Origin and Development. 1977, Hamilton, IL: Dandant

    Google Scholar 

  62. Grozinger CM, Sharabash NM, Whitfield CW, Robinson GE: Pheromone-mediated gene expression in the honey bee brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003, 100: 14519-14525. 10.1073/pnas.2335884100.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Hoover SER, Keeling CI, Winston ML, Slessor KN: The effect of queen pheromones on worker honey bee ovary development. Naturwissenschaften. 2003, 90: 477-480. 10.1007/s00114-003-0462-z.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Grozinger CM, Robinson GE: Endocrine modulation of a pheromone-responsive gene in the honey bee brain. J Comp Physiol. 2007, 193: 461-470. 10.1007/s00359-006-0202-x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Breed MD, Leger EA, Pearce AN, Wang YJ: Comb wax effects on the ontogeny of honey bee nestmate recognition. Anim Behav. 1998, 55: 13-20. 10.1006/anbe.1997.0581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Imler JL, Tauszia S, Jouanguy E, Forestier C, Hoffmann JA: LPS-induced immune response in Drosophila. J Endotoxin Res. 2000, 6: 459-462.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Korner P, Schmid-Hempel P: In vivo dynamics of an immune response in the bumble bee Bombus terrestris. J Invertebr Pathol. 2004, 87: 59-66. 10.1016/j.jip.2004.07.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Evans JD: Beepth: an ordered quantitative-PCR array for exploring honey bee immunity and disease. J Invertebr Pathol. 2006, 93: 135-139. 10.1016/j.jip.2006.04.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Whitfield CW, Cziko AM, Robinson G: Gene expression profiles in the brain predict behavior in individual honey bees. Science. 2003, 302: 296-299. 10.1126/science.1086807.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Sokal RR, Rholf J: Biometry. 1981, San Francisco: Freeman, WH, 2

    Google Scholar 

  71. Reyment: Compositional data analysis. Terra Rev. 1989, 1: 29-37. 10.1111/j.1365-3121.1989.tb00322.x.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Coby Schal and Michael Roe for use of their GC and GC/MS instruments, Lillian King for her assistance in the behavioral test observations of individual bees, and Joe Flowers for expert beekeeping assistance. We would also like to thank Kevin Donohue, Toshi Nojima, Dorit Eliyahu, Elsa Youngsteadt for their assistance in the GC set up and Coby Schal for interesting discussions. We would also like to thank Gene Robinson for reading an earlier version of the manuscript. This work was partially supported by funding from a USDA-NRI grant to CMG and FJR (2007-02284). FJR was supported by post-doctoral fellowships from the Conseil Regional d'Indre et Loire, France and the North Carolina State University WM Keck Center for Behavioral Biology.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to F-J Richard.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

Behavioral tests, cuticular hydrocarbons analysis and gene expression analysis were performed by FJR. Statistical analyses were performed by FJR and AA. FJR, AA, and CMG were involved in the interpretation of the data and preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Richard, FJ., Aubert, A. & Grozinger, C. Modulation of social interactions by immune stimulation in honey bee, Apis mellifera, workers . BMC Biol 6, 50 (2008). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1741-7007-6-50

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1741-7007-6-50

Keywords